Guest guest Posted April 17, 2010 Report Share Posted April 17, 2010 Interesting NYTimes article on how evolution in laboratory animals may be affecting the results of study on them, including life extension studies: Laboratory Life By OLIVIA JUDSON Here's a problem: evolution never stops. Imagine you're a wild fruit fly, of the species Drosophila melanogaster. You're happily feasting on some yeast that's growing on rotting fruit when, whoomf, you get sucked into a bottle and taken to a laboratory. From now on, this is your home. Life in a bottle — or cage — is different from life in the wild. In nature, for example, fruit flies reproduce throughout their adult lives. Often, in the laboratory, they do not: flies grown in bottles may only be allowed to reproduce for the first five or six days after emerging from the pupa. (Wild flies can live for more than 80 days.) In nature, flies choose their mates. Often, in the laboratory, they do not: they are often assigned to one, and that one may be a close relative. On top of that, the food is different; infectious diseases are rare; predators are absent. In short, the pressures of daily life have been transformed — and traits that were an advantage Out There may no longer be so Inside. Similarly, traits that would have killed you in the wild may help you get along inside a bottle. If, for example, older flies are never allowed to reproduce, the ability to lay eggs later in life becomes irrelevant, so there's nothing to prevent the appearance of mutations that interfere with that ability. Indeed, if those mutations increase early fertility, they may even be favored: the most fecund young flies are likely to leave the most descendants. Thus, the switch from the wild to the laboratory immediately alters the evolutionary trajectory of a population — and sure enough, within a few generations, laboratory-bred life-forms become noticeably different from their wild cousins. Exactly what happens depends on how the organisms are kept — different rearing methods create different evolutionary forces. But in general, laboratory Drosophila melanogaster evolve shorter lifespans than wild flies; they become less able to cope with stresses like starvation or desiccation; and their pattern of fertility changes. As you'd expect, females reared in bottles evolve to be hugely fecund as young flies but much less so when they are older. Also as you'd expect, laboratory evolution is not unique to Drosophila melanogaster. In the wasp Nasonia vitripennis, females descended from a long line of laboratory wasps evolve to be more prone to promiscuous sexual behavior than wild wasps. In the Mediterranean fruit fly, Ceratitis capitata, laboratory-reared females evolve to be less fussy about who they mate with, and male sexiness changes. Wild female medflies don't find laboratory-reared males as attractive as they find wild males. Mexican fruit flies, Anastrepha ludens, have the same problem: laboratory males have evolved in such a way that they are less popular with wild females. Mice show a host of changes, too. Compared to their wild relations, laboratory mice are typically bigger, more docile, reach sexual maturity earlier and die younger. Some of these changes can appear quickly: one study found that the ability to reproduce later in life declined within 10 generations of the mice being bred in the laboratory. Intriguingly, laboratory mice also have longer telomeres than wild mice. (Telomeres are the segments of DNA at the ends of chromosomes; they are thought to play a role in aging and cancer.) Since no one is deliberately breeding mice for extra-long telomeres, this must arise as some consequence of laboratory life. But what? That's not clear. One possibility is that it's due to inbreeding — for lab mice are often highly inbred. Consistent with this, one study of white-footed mice, Peromyscus leucopus, found that, when animals were forced to inbreed, telomeres lengthened substantially in fewer than 30 generations — although why this should be so is entirely mysterious. All of which is fascinating. But does it matter? That depends. For some scientific problems, the fact that laboratory life-forms evolve substantial differences from their wild relatives is irrelevant. For others, however, it matters a lot. Let me give you two examples. Adaptation to the laboratory — or to captivity more generally — can make it much more difficult for organisms to thrive if they are later released to the wild. This has important implications for the conservation of endangered animals and for the control of pests. Captive breeding programs have been important tools for re-establishing wild populations of species such as the California condor; but not all programs are successful. Genetic changes in captivity may be one reason. Similarly, many pest control programs depend on the " sterile male technique, " whereby males are bred in the laboratory, sterilized, then released into nature to mate with wild females. For this to work, the wild females must find the laboratory males attractive. Changes in mating behavior like the ones I mentioned earlier can, therefore, quickly reduce the effectiveness of the approach. A second area where laboratory evolution can be a serious problem is in the study of subjects like the evolution of aging, and the diseases associated with it. For example, the study of laboratory populations may give a misleading impression of how easy it is to extend lifespans: since laboratory organisms tend to have unnaturally short lifespans, discovering ways to make them live longer may not be especially informative. We may simply be reversing the unnatural shortening that we created in the first place, a view supported by the fact that selection to increase lifespan in laboratory populations often simply restores it to levels seen in the wild. Such realizations have led an increasing number of scientists to argue that long-established laboratory populations are " suspect starting material " for understanding aging, and that comparisons with wild populations " support the pessimistic interpretation that laboratory-adapted stocks of rodents may be particularly inappropriate for the analysis of the genetic and physiological factors that regulate aging in mammals. " For some subjects, it's better to go wild. http://opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2010/04/13/laboratory-life/ Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.