Jump to content
RemedySpot.com

New on PTManager Blog

Rate this topic


Guest guest

Recommended Posts

Your Daily Posterous Spaces Update October 10th, 2011 Three major

cognitive errors physicians

make<http://ptmanagerblog.com/three-major-cognitive-errors-physicians-make>

Posted about 17 hours ago by [image: _portrait_thumb] Kovacek,

PT, DPT, MSA <http://posterous.com/people/1l1oCkDWEWjv> to

PTManager<http://ptmanagerblog.com>

[image: Like this

post]<http://posterous.com/likes/create?post_id=74778266>

Three major cognitive errors physicians make

by Jerome Groopman, MD and Pamela Hartzband,

MD<http://www.kevinmd.com/blog/post-author/jerome-groopman-and-pamela-hartzband>\

|

in

Physician <http://www.kevinmd.com/blog/category/physician> |

http://www.kevinmd.com/blog/?p=59906 "

As physicians, we all dread missing a diagnosis: indigestion that turns out

to be angina, back pain that signals an aortic aneurysm, migraine that

proves to be a brain tumor. Although it is only an estimate, several studies

in the medical literature indicate that misdiagnosis occurs in 15% to 20% of

all cases, and in half of these, there is serious harm to the patient.

Researchers have found that the vast majority of misdiagnoses, about 80%,

are due to cognitive errors. Can thinking about your thinking help prevent

these errors?

In our work as physicians, we are prone to make three major cognitive

errors.

*Three ‘A’ errors*

“Anchoring” occurs when we fix on a particular bit of information or data

given to us, and then think in a constrained, linear way. This causes us to

potentially fail to obtain other information about a problem and to proceed

down only one path of investigation.

A physician can anchor on a specific aspect of the history, a physical

finding or a laboratory result. For example, we wrote about a case of

anchoring in our November 2008

column<http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2008/11/mindful.htm>,

“Anchoring errors ensue when diagnoses get lost in translation,” where a

patient’s complaint of gas caused clinicians to initially miss an abdominal

aneurysm. Another patient who said he had a lack of “stamina” underwent an

extensive and fruitless evaluation involving multiple blood tests and scans

until a physician asked what exactly “stamina” meant.

“Availability” is another common thinking trap. Here, we are strongly

influenced by dramatic or unusual cases that are prominent in our memory and

easily recalled, and thus “available” when we consider a new patient’s

problem. As physicians, we are all swayed by the dramatic cases we have

seen, and may too quickly conclude that the symptoms or findings before us

correspond to those that were present in the prior case.

We saw the power of availability in a case of Crohn’s disease with initial

symptoms of weight loss and fatigue. Each specialist who saw the patient

immediately considered diagnoses in their own particular field, akin to the

famous poem about the blind men and the elephant presented in our April 2010

column <http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2010/4/minful.htm>, “Seeing the

whole diagnostic picture.” But availability can sometimes work to the

doctor’s advantage, as with a clinician who discussed in our September 2009

column <http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2009/09/mindful.htm>,

“Unmasking the patient’s hidden agenda,” how he had recently led a focus

group on depression. With this discussion fresh in his mind, he probed more

deeply than usual a patient’s denial of despair, and thus averted a

potential suicide.

The third major thinking trap is an “attribution error.” This occurs when a

physician is overly influenced by certain personal characteristics,

particularly those that correspond to social stereotypes. The doctor then

fits the history, physical findings and laboratory studies into a preset

conception about that person, rather than weighing the information in a

dispassionate way.

When we attribute findings to the social or other characteristics of the

patient we fail to consider that this might be misleading. An attribution

error was evident in the case of an elderly woman with failure to thrive

that was diagnosed as just “old age.” In fact, her poor food intake and

weight loss were due to masseter claudication from temporal arteritis, as

described in our May 2009

column<http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2011/05/mindful.htm>,

“It’s just old age—or is it? Don’t be guided by stereotypes.” Another case

of attribution error involved a woman described in our July/August 2010

column <http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2011/07/mindful.htm>,

“Attribution error confounds a diagnosis after colon cancer.” The woman was

assumed to be nutritionally replete because she was obese, but in fact was

deficient, lacking thiamine.

While attribution errors usually arise from negative stereotypes, there are

also positive stereotypes that can mislead us. We devoted our May 2011

column <http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2011/05/mindful.htm>,

“Attribution error results from a positive stereotype,” to the case of a man

with diabetes who was so deeply educated about his disease that his doctors

assumed he knew not to reinject the same body site repeatedly, when in fact

he was doing just that; this resulted in lipodystrophy and impaired insulin

absorption that presented as apparent insulin resistance. An attribution

error was also involved in our October 2010

column<http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2010/10/mindful.htm>,

“When patients don’t tell all: The diagnostic challenge,” in assuming that a

wealthy businessman in India could not have leprosy as a cause of his

neuropathy since this was a disease of the poor.

While these three “A’s” of anchoring, availability and attribution are the

most common, there are other cognitive pitfalls.

*Confirmation bias*

“Confirmation bias” involves ignoring or rationalizing contradictory data to

make the pieces of the puzzle fit neatly into the presumed picture. An

unusual complaint or laboratory finding is dismissed in our minds as an

“outlier” when it should actually raise a red flag, indicating that our

presumption may be incorrect. Confirmation bias was prominent in a case of

hypothyroidism occurring in a physician, where an elevated creatine

phosphokinase level was initially ignored, as shown in our January 2009

column <http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2009/01/mindful.htm>, “Perils

of diagnosing the physician-patient.”

*Satisfaction of search*

During medical school and residency training, many of us learned about

Ockham’s razor. This principle, derived from medieval scholars, holds that

we should try to find a single unifying explanation for a diverse

constellation of clues about a patient’s problem.

While it is valuable to look for a single cause that may explain all of the

symptoms, physical findings and laboratory studies, we should keep in mind

that in the real world, patients may not adhere to Ockham’s razor. There may

be multiple maladies occurring concurrently, and we should not immediately

be satisfied in our search when we identify one. This trap of “satisfaction

of search” is particularly prominent in radiology, where studies show that

once a radiologist has identified an abnormality on an X-ray or scan, his or

her mind tends to neglect other findings that might be important and

indicate more than a single pathological process at work. We wrote about a

radiologist who noted a renal tumor on abdominal imaging and then halted his

search so that appendicitis was not detected early in “Beware of ‘search

satisfaction,’ a common cognitive error” from the May 2008

column<http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2008/05/five.htm>.

We also discussed the concept in our March 2008

column<http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2008/03/three.htm>,

“Patient’s doubts about diagnosis prompt a second opinion,” in a case of

hypercalcemia in the setting of myeloma where the patient also had primary

hyperparathyroidism.

*Representativeness error*

“Representativeness” or “prototype error” occurs when a case is not typical

and therefore may elude physician thinking based on pattern recognition. We

illustrated this in a patient with ’s disease that lacked classical

findings in our July/August 2011

column<http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2010/07/mindful.htm>,

“Priming to diagnose an atypical case, avoid representativeness.” Our

January 2010 column<http://www.acpinternist.org/archives/2011/01/mindful.htm>,

“What to do when one expects everything to fit, but it doesn’t,” discussed

how another clinician avoided this pitfall in his diagnosis of an

androgen-producing tumor in the ovary that was difficult to detect.

Finally, several cognitive errors may occur at different points along a

diagnostic process, and act to reinforce each other in swaying our thinking

so we arrive at an incorrect diagnosis.

*Remedies available*

Despite the power of these pitfalls, there are some remedies. We have

proposed in prior columns a few simple questions that a clinician can ask

himself or herself to protect against falling into one of these thinking

traps.

The first is: What else could it be? This allows us to unhinge if we have

anchored, to move away from a dramatic memory in the setting of

availability, and to reduce the impact of stereotype in the setting of

attribution error.

A second question is: Does anything not fit? This is a safeguard against

confirmation bias, whereby we instruct ourselves to focus on a contradictory

or discrepant finding rather than to dismiss it as an outlier and

irrelevant.

And last: Could there be more than one process at work? This contradicts

Ockham’s razor, and ensures that we are not overly parsimonious in our

deliberation. We should always consider whether there may be more than one

illness that is contributing to the symptoms and findings in our patients,

so we are not too quickly satisfied in our search. Each of these questions

helps us to keep an open mind.

Cognitive scientists designate two “systems” of thinking: intuitive and

deliberative. Clinicians use both and each has its advantages, but also

limitations. Intuitive thinking is rapid and efficient but may cause us to

miss some important clues; this kind of thinking becomes more accurate as we

accrue more experience. Deliberative thinking is slow but can sometimes help

us to see clues that do not register intuitively. Optimally, we merge both

systems when we develop a differential diagnosis.

With the time pressure and hectic nature of modern health care, there is, we

believe, great value in pausing to reflect on our thinking, particularly

when an initial presumption about a diagnosis appears not to succeed in

explaining the condition or an empiric therapy does not ameliorate the

patient’s symptoms. At these times, drawing on both intuitive and

deliberative thinking and asking the above questions can be vital in

avoiding thinking traps and moving us back onto a better diagnostic path.

via

kevinmd.com<http://www.kevinmd.com/blog/2011/10/major-cognitive-errors-physician\

s.html>

15 Biggest Hospital False Claims and Anti-Kickback Stories of 2011 |

Stark Act / Antikickback Statute / False

Claims<http://ptmanagerblog.com/15-biggest-hospital-false-claims-and-anti-kic>

Posted about 16 hours ago by [image: _portrait_thumb] Kovacek,

PT, DPT, MSA <http://posterous.com/people/1l1oCkDWEWjv> to

PTManager<http://ptmanagerblog.com>

[image: Like this

post]<http://posterous.com/likes/create?post_id=74786535>

http://www.beckershospitalreview.com/stark-act-/-antikickback-statute-/-false-cl\

aims/15-biggest-hospital-false-claims-and-anti-kickback-stories-of-2011.html#.Tp\

H8MRGZXak.gmail

Reimbursement 201 Managing and Tracking Reimbursement

Video<http://ptmanagerblog.com/reimbursement-201-managing-and-tracking-reimb>

Posted about 12 hours ago by [image: _portrait_thumb] Kovacek,

PT, DPT, MSA <http://posterous.com/people/1l1oCkDWEWjv> to

PTManager<http://ptmanagerblog.com>

[image: Like this

post]<http://posterous.com/likes/create?post_id=74806966>

Now available online

*Reimbursement 201 Managing and Tracking Reimbursement*

Originally presented at Michigan Physical Therapy Association Oct 2011.

Enjoy!

You can watch it here:

Embedded media -- click here to see

it.<http://ptmanagerblog.com/reimbursement-201-managing-and-tracking-reimb>

[image: Reimbursement 201 Managing and Tracking

Reimbursement]<http://vimeo.com/30285460>

*Reimbursement 201 Managing and Tracking Reimbursement*

Embedded media -- click here to see

it.<http://ptmanagerblog.com/reimbursement-201-managing-and-tracking-reimb>

" Part 2 of a Six Part Series on Reimbursement "

Involves Kovacek, PT, DPT, MSA <http://vimeo.com/user3273111>.

Forward this email to your friends and family so that they can see it, too.

Don't want these alerts anymore?

http://vimeo.com/settings/notifications

LOVE,

[image: vimeo]

[image: Posterous] <http://posterous.com> Want your

own?<http://posterous.com> Change

your email

settings<http://posterous.com/email_subscriptions/hash/gspsqucxgqviGogjvCufJwAxB\

xkgmH>

Link to comment
Share on other sites

Join the conversation

You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.

Guest
Reply to this topic...

×   Pasted as rich text.   Paste as plain text instead

  Only 75 emoji are allowed.

×   Your link has been automatically embedded.   Display as a link instead

×   Your previous content has been restored.   Clear editor

×   You cannot paste images directly. Upload or insert images from URL.

Loading...
×
×
  • Create New...