Guest guest Posted June 10, 2009 Report Share Posted June 10, 2009 WOWOWOWOW! Fantatic...I loved it...as a Brain Gym® teacher this interests me vey much...thank you > From: kiddietalk <kiddietalk@...> > Subject: [ ] Scientific American article How mirror neurons let us interact with others > > Date: Tuesday, June 9, 2009, 7:10 PM > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > Mind Matters - June 9, 2009 > > Mirroring Behavior > > How mirror neurons let us interact with others > > > > By Lametti > > > > Eighteen years ago, in a laboratory at the University of > Parma in Italy, a neuroscientist named Giacomo Rizzolatti > and his graduate students were recording electrical activity > from neurons in the brain of a macaque monkey. It was a > typical study in neurophysiology: needle thin electrodes ran > into the monkey's head through a small window cut out of > its skull; the tips of the electrodes were placed within > individual neurons in a brain region called the premotor > cortex. At the time, the premotor cortex was known to be > involved in the planning and initiation of movements, and, > just as Rizzolatti expected, when the monkey moved its arm > to grab an object the electrodes signaled that premotor > neurons were firing. And then, neglecting to turn off their > equipment, Rizzolatti and his team got lunch. > > > > What followed lunch that day was a serendipitous discovery. > One of Rizzolatti's graduate students decided to have an > ice cream cone for dessert, which he ate in full view of the > wired-up monkey. To his surprise, the electrodes suddenly > began to signal a spike in cellular activity in the premotor > cortex, even though the monkey was motionless. > > > > After shoveling more food into their mouths, the scientists > determined that some of the neurons they were recording from > fired when the monkey made certain movements – like > bringing a piece of food to its mouth – and when the > monkey watched someone make similar movements. In other > words, the neurons were mirroring observed actions: when the > monkey watched someone perform an action its brain seemed to > be simulating neural activity as if the monkey was > performing the action itself. These " mirror > neurons " , as Rizzolatti later dubbed them, were > hypothesized to constitute a brain system responsible for > our ability to understand the actions of others. We know > about our world because we've interacted with it and > only by simulating this interaction in our heads can we > comprehend the behaviour of someone else. > > > > In 1992, almost a year after the ice cream cone incident, > Rizzolatti published a short report in a minor neuroscience > journal describing his discovery of mirror neurons. The > paper was largely ignored. Then, almost four years later, he > published a more detailed account of the finding in the > journal Brain that launched a torrent of research – more > than 300 papers in the past ten years – into mirror > neurons and their properties. As he explained to The New > York Times in an interview, " It took us several years > to believe what we were seeing. " > > > > Since Rizzolatti's 1996 paper, studies in primates and > humans support the idea that mirror neurons help us > understand observed behaviour. One primate study found that > mirror neurons were activated simply by the sound of an > action, like the ripping of paper, while another found that > the mental representation of actions was enough to cause > mirror neuron firing. These are important results because > they demonstrate a mirror neuron response to the meaning of > an action and not just the observation of one. > > > > In humans, brain imaging studies of people with autism – > a deficit characterized by an inability to comprehend > observed behavior – have shown that autistics have less > activity in premotor regions during the observation of > actions than normal subjects. And more recently, in a > dramatic example of the importance of mirror neurons in our > understanding of others, apraxia patients with cortical > damage in mirror neuron areas were shown to have difficulty > recognizing whether hand gestures, like sticking out a thumb > to hitch a ride, were performed correctly. > > > > A new paper by Vittorio Caggiano and colleagues at the > University of Tubingen in Germany – Rizzolatti is a > co-author - suggests that mirror neurons might also play a > role in helping us choose appropriate responses to behaviors > we observe. Using similar methods to the original mirror > neuron studies in macaques, the latest paper found that some > mirror neurons fired when a monkey watched the experimenter > grasp an object within its reach, while other mirror neurons > fired when the monkey watched the experimenter grasp an > object that was out of its reach. These mirror neurons, it > seemed, responded differently to observed behavior depending > on how far the behavior occurred from the monkey. > > > > The authors of the paper then repeated the experiment but > with an important twist: they placed a neck-high wall in > front of the monkey, forcing the animal to make an > intermediate movement – a reach over the wall in this case > – to grasp any object. In this situation, the mirror > neurons that responded before only to the observation of an > action within the monkey's reach completely stopped > firing. And the mirror neurons that responded only to the > observation of an action outside of the monkey's reach > now fired vigorously, no matter where the experimenter > grasped the object. The mirror neurons were not simply > encoding the distance of an observed act, they were encoding > whether the monkey could perform the act without an > intermediate behavior. > > > > One can only speculate, of course, as to the behavioral > consequences of mirror neurons that differentiate between > actions that can be immediately performed and actions that > require intermediate behaviors. But an intriguing idea > proposed by the paper's authors is that such a system > might help us choose behaviors of our own. If mirror neurons > help us understand the act of a basketball player making a > jump shot by simulating neural activity as if we were making > the jump shot, perhaps, if we don't have a basketball > within reach, they run simulations of behaviors that might > allow us to get the ball – calling for a pass or grabbing > a rebound. Such neural simulations might help us decide how > best to quickly respond to changing events. > > > > Determining whether such mirror neurons exist in humans > will require more research, using indirect methods such as > functional brain imaging. Even so, this finding suggests how > mirror neurons might link the understanding of the behavior > of other people with the production of our own behavior, > which is a crucial step towards determining the neural > processes that caused Rizzolatti's graduate student to > eat an ice cream cone on that fateful day in Italy, eighteen > years ago. > > > > Are you a scientist? Have you recently read a peer-reviewed > paper that you want to write about? Then contact Mind > Matters editor Jonah Lehrer, the science writer behind the > blog The Frontal Cortex and the book Proust Was a > Neuroscientist. His latest book is How We Decide. > > > > ===== > > Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.