Jump to content
RemedySpot.com

Autistic Toddlers Prefer to Gaze at Geometric Patterns: Study

Rate this topic


Guest guest

Recommended Posts

Executive Health September 06, 2010, 16:00 EST

Autistic Toddlers Prefer to Gaze at Geometric Patterns: Study

Finding could help spot the disorder early, when treatments are most effective,

researchers say

By Jenifer Goodwin

HealthDay Reporter

MONDAY, Sept. 6 (HealthDay News) -- When given the choice to gaze at geometric

patterns or children dancing and playing, toddlers with autism spent more time

looking at the patterns while typically developing toddlers preferred to look at

other kids, a new study finds.

The finding could be another clue to helping doctors and parents spot the

disorder early, when treatment can be most effective, experts said.

In the study, researchers showed 110 toddlers ages 14 months to 3.5 years old

two video screens, each of which was simultaneously playing a one-minute video.

One video was of " screensavers " that featured moving geometric shapes and

patterns; the other video was of children dancing, jumping, smiling and playing.

About 37 of the children had either been diagnosed with an autism spectrum

disorder or were later diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder; 22 of the

children had another developmental delay, while 51 were developing in the usual

way.

While children viewed the videos, researchers used an " eye tracker " -- a tiny

infrared beam bounced off the lens of the toddlers' eyes -- to measure where the

children focused their gaze.

About 40 percent of children who had been diagnosed with autism or who were

later diagnosed with autism spent more than half of the time staring at the

geometric patterns, while only one (less than 2 percent) of the typically

developing toddlers preferred the geometric patterns.

About 9 percent of children with developmental delays preferred the geometric

patterns.

All of the children who showed the strongest preference for the geometric

pattern -- that is, they gazed at it more than 69 percent of the time -- had

autism, according to the study.

" Only autistic babies looked at the geometric patterns more than 69 percent of

the time. No normal babies did at all, " said lead study author Pierce, an

assistant professor of neuroscience at University of California, San Diego, and

clinical research director at the UCSD Autism Center of Excellence. " It's pretty

clear that showing heightened interest in geometric patterns and repetitive

moving objects is a risk factor for autism. "

The study is published online Sept. 6 in the Archives of General Psychiatry.

Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by problems with social

interaction, verbal and nonverbal communication and restricted interests and

behaviors.

While many children are not diagnosed until after age 3, interest is growing in

uncovering the early signs of the disorder so that children can receive

treatment sooner, when it may be the most beneficial, said Landa,

director of the Center for Autism & Related Disorders at Kennedy Krieger

Institute.

She said it would be valuable to follow the 60 percent of autistic children who

did not show a preference for the geometric patterns at the time of the study,

to determine if later on they did, or if they continued to prefer the more

social images, she said.

Another question is whether early intervention would cause the autistic children

to become more social, she added.

" It's a really neat study, and the findings make a lot of sense, " Landa said.

" Autism is heterogenous. Some with autism are aloof. Others with autism are

social, but they are socially unusual in their behavior. There is still a lot

more digging that needs to be done to understand the children in the autism

spectrum disorder group that didn't prefer the geometric patterns. "

While there is no one sign that's a clear indicator of autism, parents may want

to pay attention if they notice their toddlers fixated on things like spinning

fans for long periods of time, or spinning the wheels of a toy car, or flicking

the eyelids of a baby doll, or other repetitive behaviors, Pierce said.

The study also found that autistic children had fewer saccades, or eye

movements, while looking at the geometric patterns than the normally developing

children did while looking at the social images. " It was as if the patterns had

a hypnotic effect, " Pierce said.

On the other hand, when looking at the social images, the autistic kids had more

saccades than the normally developing or developmentally delayed toddlers.

The UCSD finding comes on the heels of another study, released Friday by Landa's

team at Kennedy Kreiger, that also looked at the early signs of autism. It found

that infants at high risk of autism were less likely to spontaneously look at

their parents than other infants.

In the study, Landa and her colleagues observed 25 six-month-old babies who had

an autistic sibling and 25 infants with no family history of autism.

Infant siblings of children with autism are 25 times more likely to develop

autism, according to the study in the September issue of the Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry.

Both sets of infants were equally likely to look at their parent when the parent

tried to get their attention, Landa said.

But the babies at high risk of autism were less likely to look over at their

parents when unprompted and spent more time fixated on toys or a joystick used

in the experiment.

" This is about social initiation, " Landa said. " The baby siblings of children

with autism looked less often and with less duration. It's something parents

should keep an eye on. "

More information

There's more on autism at the U.S. National Institute of Neurological Disorders

and Stroke.

SOURCES: Pierce, Ph.D. assistant professor, neuroscience, University of

California, San Diego and director of clinical research, UCSD Autism Center of

Excellence, La Jolla, Calif.; Landa, Ph.D, director of Center for Autism

& Related Disorders, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, Md.; Archives of

General Psychiatry, Sept. 6, 2010; Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

September 2010.

Link to comment
Share on other sites

Join the conversation

You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.

Guest
Reply to this topic...

×   Pasted as rich text.   Paste as plain text instead

  Only 75 emoji are allowed.

×   Your link has been automatically embedded.   Display as a link instead

×   Your previous content has been restored.   Clear editor

×   You cannot paste images directly. Upload or insert images from URL.

Loading...
×
×
  • Create New...